The debate over how working memory works

The debate over how working memory works

The debate over how working memory works 150 150 icnagency

By: David Orenstein | Picower Institute for Learning and Memory | The debate over how working memory works | MIT News – Neuroscience

In a debate where the stakes are nothing short of understanding how the brain maintains its so-called “sketchpad of conscious thought,” researchers discuss exactly what makes working memory work in dueling papers in the Aug. 8 edition of the Journal of Neuroscience.

Working memory is how you hold things in mind like the directions to a new restaurant and the list of specials the waiter rattles off after you sit down. Given that working memory capacity is a strong correlate of intelligence and that its dysfunction is a major symptom in common psychiatric disorders such as schizophrenia and autism, Mikael Lundqvist, a postdoc at MIT’s Picower Institute for Learning and Memory and lead author of one of the papers, says it’s important that the field achieve a true understanding of how it works.

Lundqvist’s corresponding author and Picower Professor Earl Miller adds that if scientist can figure out how working memory works, “we can figure out how to fix it.”

“Working memory is the sketchpad of consciousness,” Miller says. “Doesn’t everyone want to know how our conscious mind works?”

The opposing paper in the “Dual Perspectives” section of the journal is led by Christos Constantinidis of the Wake Forest School of Medicine.

The central issue of the debate is what happens after you hear or see what you need to remember and must then hold or control it in mind to apply it later. During that interim, or delay period, the central question is whether neurons in your brain’s prefrontal cortex maintain it by persistently firing away, like an idling car engine, or whether they spike in brief but coordinated bursts to store and retrieve information via the patterns of their connections, which is akin to how longer-term memory works.

In their essay, Lundqvist, Miller, and Pawel Herman off the KTH Royal Institute of Technology in Stockholm, Sweden take the latter position. They argue that brief, coordinated bursts are clearly evident in the observations of the most recent experiments and that such activity can more satisfactorily produce key attributes of working memory, including efficient, independent control of multiple items with precise timing.

Importantly, the idea that spiking during the delay period drives changes in neural connections, or synapses, reinforces the classic idea that spiking has a crucial role in working memory, Miller says. The disagreement, he says, is merely that the spiking activity is not as persistent as it looks in older experiments.

“We’re showing additional mechanisms by which spiking maintains working memory and gives volitional control,” Miller says. “Our work doesn’t argue against the idea that delay activity spiking plays a role in working memory, it adds further support. We are just saying that at a more granular level, there are some additional things going on.”

For example, much of the disagreement arises from how different researchers have collected and analyzed their data. The data supporting the persistence interpretation arise mostly from analyses in which researchers averaged the firing patterns of small numbers of neurons over many working memory trials, the MIT authors say.

Averages, however, tend to smooth data out over the long term. Instead, in newer experiments, scientists have analyzed the spiking of many neurons in each individual trial. There, it’s clear that as animals perform working memory tasks, populations of neurons fire in brief, coordinated bursts, Miller and Lundqvist say.

In their research, members of the Miller lab have also shown how groups of neurons are coordinated, demonstrating how a large-scale, precisely timed interplay of brain rhythms correlate with goal-directed control of working memory functions such as storing or releasing, information from being in mind.

Some of the disagreement also arises from models of working memory function. Miller and Lundqvist argue that it makes functional sense that neurons fire in short, cohesive bursts in accord with circuit-wide oscillations. That uses less energy than keeping neurons firing all the time, for example, and readily explains how multiple items can be held in mind simultaneously (distinct bursts representing different pieces of information can occur at different times). Moreover, storing information in patterns of synaptic connections makes the information more resilient to distraction than if neurons are constantly trying to maintain it through activity.

“Storing information with a mixture of spiking and synapses gives the brain more flexibility,” Lundqvist says. “It can juggle the activation of different memories, allowing the brain to hold multiple memories without them interfering with each other. Plus, synapses can store temporarily store memories while the spiking processes other thoughts.

“This could explain how our working memory is not erased by things that temporarily distract us,” he says.

With a lot of new research activity and data coming in, Lundqvist added, it’s a debate whose time has come.

“This is a good time to see what the evidence is and to determine what are the experiments that will settle this,” he says. “We need more experiments to settle this. They will give us not only more insight into this question of persistence but also about working memory function.”

MIT paper recommends four major principles to help research continue to move forward: measuring the activity of whole populations of individual neurons; analyzing every trial separately; making the tasks animals do complex enough to require controlling multiple pieces of information; and measuring neural rhythms instead of just spiking.

The Miller lab’s research on working memory is funded by the National Institutes of Health, the Office of Naval Research, and the MIT Picower Institute Innovation Fund.

icn-neurocience

MIT News is dedicated to communicating to the media and the public the news and achievements of the students, faculty, staff and the greater MIT community.
Back to top